Friday, December 5, 2014

Favorites in Social Psych: Concepts and Psychologists

Favorite concepts
1.      One of my favorite concepts from social psych is Cognitive Dissonance Theory, which is the tendency for individuals to want and to maintain consistency between their attitudes and behaviors (Festinger, 1957).  When their attitudes and behaviors don’t match, individuals feel discomfort, or dissonance, and attempt to relieve it by changing their attitudes.  For instance, if you are trying to start an exercise routine and skip one day, you might try to justify it by thinking, “I need a rest day” or “I am too tired to work out!”  In fact, when we learned about cognitive dissonance theory on the first day of class, I liked it so much that I named my blog after it. J
2.      Another one of my favorite concepts is the mindlessness phenomenon, which is the tendency for individuals to comply with other’s requests just because they say “because” (Langer, 1978).  For example, Langer and colleagues performed an experiment where they asked individuals to cut in line at the copy machine.  They either asked without justification (e.g., “May I use the copy machine”) or asked with justification.  In addition, they split the justification condition into two categories: reasonable (e.g., “…because I’m in a rush”) or unreasonable (e.g., “…because I have to make copies”).  Regardless of the reason, people complied more so when a reason was given than not.  Surprisingly, people complied equally when there was justification!! In other words, 94% complied when asked “…because I’m in a rush” and 93% complied when asked “…because I have to make copies.” I think that mindlessness is just so fascinating because people comply with (in my opinion) a pretty stupid request just because they say “because”!
3.      Universal male is a concept of how sexes are presented in society.  It basically assumes that males are the “universal standard” to which females are compared and are somewhat “other” or “unnatural” (Tavris, 1992).  I really like this concept! I think it helps explain a situation I experienced in high school:  I was in the math club (nerdy, haha!) and I ended up winning a few math contests.  At our graduate recognition ceremony, the head of the math club (a white, male professor) remarked about how I “broke into the boy’s club,” insinuating that women were somewhat worse than men, in terms of mathematics aptitude.  Of course, (I think) he meant this as a compliment, meaning I achieved above and beyond (because women “aren’t as good as math”).  The universal male helped to explain a lot of my frustration as a woman in the sciences, who just happens to be good at math.
4.      The matching hypothesis is a tendency for individuals to be attracted to others who are similar to them in physical attractiveness (Feingold, 1988).  I think it’s interesting how individuals “end up” with someone of similar attractiveness, like the activity we did in class!  (I actually did one of my research methods projects on this topic so this may be effort justification talking. J)
5.      Psychological reactance is one of the coolest concepts.  It is the tendency for individuals to rebel or react against others when their freedom is threatened (Brehm & Brehm, 1981).  I think this is so interesting because I think it explains a lot of my arguments with my parents as a teenager.  Just being told not to do something made me want to do it more For instance, my dad always told me not to stay out past midnight and often I’d stay out until 12:30! (Sorry, mom and dad, it was psychological reactance!)

Favorite social psychologists
1.      I think Robert Cialdini is really cool! I really think his research on persuasive techniques is so fascinating! I also like how he did “undercover” research working in sales!
2.      Leon Festinger also tops my list of cool social psychologists, simply because he theorized cognitive dissonance theory.  I like how he tried to design the most boring possible task and people said it was interesting, just because of dissonance!  I also like social comparison theory because it is just so, so simple!
3.      Elliott Aronson did a lot of really cool studies.  I really like the Jigsaw Classroom study! In addition, I thought his studies on cognitive dissonance. The toy study on insufficient deterrence has a ton of implications for changing behavior! And the sex study on effort justification is just kind of funny.  I think it was cleverly designed (like Festinger’s peg-turning study) and it is funny that people would respond that a discussion of earthworm sex was interesting!
4.      I really liked Muzafer Sherif’s Robbers cave study.  I think it has so many implications for solving group conflict by making a superordinate goal! I also really like his study on the autokinetic effect and conformity!
5.      Since we learned about his experiments in principles, I thought that Stanley Milgram was really clever.  I think it is so interesting how people obeyed, even though they believed that they were harming another individual!  Learning more about it was especially interesting, in light of what I learned when I studied abroad in Germany and went to one of the concentration camps.  When there, I just couldn’t believe how someone could commit such horrendous acts, so I think all of the research on obedience is just really fascinating.
References
Brehm, S. S., & Brehm, J. W. (1981). Psychological reactance: A theory of freedom and control (Vol. 26). New York: Academic Press.
Feingold, A. (1988). Matching for attractiveness in romantic partners and same-sex friends: A meta-analysis and theoretical critique. Psychological Bulletin, 104(2), 226-235.
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Langer, E. J., Blank, A., & Chanowitz, B. (1978). The mindlessness of ostensibly thoughtful action: The role of" placebic" information in interpersonal interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology36(6), 635-642.

Tavris, C. (1993). The mismeasure of woman. Feminism & Psychology3(2), 149-168.

Tuesday, December 2, 2014

Proximity, Similarity, and Balance in Friendships


I met one of my closest friends during my first week of high school.  She sat next to me on the first day of our World History class.  Over the first couples of classes, we made polite small talk.  Upon news of our first quiz, we talked more about what the other was studying and agreed to study together.  Eventually, we discussed more in-depth topics and found that we shared many things in common, including similar personalities, senses of humor, and tastes in music and TV shows.
            The beginning of our friendship can be explained by multiple social psych concepts.  First, we were drawn together by the proximity effect.  The proximity effect is the tendency for individuals to be attracted to those who are close to them (Festinger, 1950).  In other words, we were just lucky enough to be in the same city, at the same high school, and in the same World History class to meet and sit next to each other.
            Following the proximity effect was the need for affiliation, which is our psychological need, as humans, to interact with and maintain relationships with others (McAdams, 1989).  As young freshmen in a new high school, we were highly motivated to seek out friendships.  Furthermore, the stress of our first quiz pushed us to interact with each other more.  In fact, humans have a tendency to seek affiliation under times of stress, in order to cope more effectively (Schacter, 1959).  So, our first quiz was a blessing in disguise because it helped us to spend time together, outside of class, studying and talking. 
            Lastly, once we found that we had many things in common, we were influenced by the similarity effect, which is the tendency for people to be attracted to others who are similar to themselves (Montoya, Horton, & Kirchner, 2008).  Because we had many things in common, our friendship was balanced.  In other words, our friendship was swayed by Balance Theory, which is a phenomenon whereby we want our friends to like what we like and dislike what we dislike (Heider, 1958).  Thus, because we had many things in common, we had little to disagree about, maintaining a balance.  (n=362)
References
Festinger, L. (1950).  Informal social communication. Psychological Review, 57, 271-282.
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley.
McAdams, D. P. (1989). Intimacy: The need to be close.  New York: Doubleday.
Montoya, R. M., Horton,, R. S., & Kirchner, J.. (2008). Is actual similarity necessary for attraction?  A meta-analysis of actual and perceived similarity.  Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 25, 889-922

Schacter, S. (1959). The psychology of affiliation: Experimental studies of the sources of gregariousness.  Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.